Multiplication and division of fractions: numerical cognition development and assessment procedures

http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8409-9973 Alfonso F. Díaz-Cardenas
Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, México., México
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5633-0867 Alfonso Díaz-Furlong
Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, México
Hector Adrian Díaz-Furlong
Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, México
M. Rayo Sankey-García
SEP Puebla., México
Gemma Zago-Portillo
SEP, México

Multiplication and division of fractions: numerical cognition development and assessment procedures

Revista Latinoamericana de Investigación en Matemática Educativa, RELIME, vol. 22, núm. 3, 2019

Comité Latinoamericano de Matemática Educativa

Cita recomendada:

Díaz-Cárdenas, A.F., Díaz-Furlong, A., Díaz-Furlong, H.A., Sankey-García, M.R. & Zago-Portillo, G.(2019). Multiplication and division of fractions: numerical cognition development and assessment procedures. En Revista Latinoamericana de Investigación en Matemática Educativa, 22(3), 333-362. https://doi.org/10.12802/relime.19.2234

Recepción: 25 Julio 2018

Aprobación: 15 Noviembre 2019

Financiamiento

Fuente: PRODEP

Nº de contrato: BUAP-PTC-457, DSA / 103.5 / 16/11069

Beneficiario: NPTC project

Financiamiento

Fuente: CONACyT

Abstract: The number and its basic operations can be conceptualised within a general system of relations. Children need to construct a system of numbers within which they can add, subtract, multiply and divide any rational number. Products and quotients can be defined in terms of general relational schemes. In this study, we examine whether elementary school children can construct a system of numbers such that fraction multiplication and division are based on the construction of general relational schemes. Groups of students are not homogeneous and children progress at different rates. For reliable assessment teachers need methods to examine developmental and individual differences in cognitive representations of mathematical concepts and operations. A logistic regression curve offers a visualisation of the learning process as a function of average marks. The analysis of fraction multiplication and division items shows an improvement on correct response probability, especially for students with a higher average mark.

Keywords: relational schemes, fraction multiplication teaching, logistic regression curve, educational development assessment.

Resumen: El número y sus operaciones básicas se pueden conceptualizar dentro de un sistema general de relaciones. Los niños necesitan construir un sistema de números dentro del cual puedan sumar, restar, multiplicar y dividir cualquier número racional. Los productos y los cocientes se pueden definir en términos de esquemas relacionales generales. En este estudio, examinamos si los niños de escuela primaria pueden construir un sistema de números tal que la multiplicación y división de fracciones se basan en la construcción de esquemas relacionales generales. Los grupos de estudiantes no son homogéneos y los niños progresan a diferentes ritmos. Para una evaluación confiable, los maestros necesitan métodos para examinar las diferencias individuales y de desarrollo en las representaciones cognitivas de los conceptos y operaciones matemáticos. Una curva de regresión logística ofrece una visualización del proceso de aprendizaje como una función de las notas promedio. El análisis de elementos de multiplicación y división de fracciones muestra una mejora en la probabilidad de respuesta correcta, especialmente para estudiantes con una calificación promedio más alta.

Palabras clave: esquemas relacionales, enseñanza de multiplicación de fracciones, curva de regresión logística, evaluación del desarrollo educativo.

Resumo: O número e suas operações básicas podem ser conceituados dentro de um sistema geral de relações. As crianças precisam construir um sistema de números dentro do qual possam somar, subtrair, multiplicar e dividir qualquer número racional. Produtos e quocientes podem ser definidos em termos de esquemas relacionais gerais. Neste estudo, examinamos se as crianças do ensino fundamental podem construir um sistema de números de tal forma que a multiplicação e divisão de frações são baseadas na construção de esquemas relacionais gerais. Grupos de estudantes não são homogêneos e as crianças progridem em taxas diferentes. Para uma avaliação confiável, os professores precisam de métodos para examinar diferenças individuais e de desenvolvimento nas representações cognitivas de conceitos e operações matemáticas. Uma curva de regressão logística oferece uma visualização do processo de aprendizagem como uma função das notas médias. A análise dos itens de multiplicação e divisão de frações mostra uma melhoria na probabilidade de resposta correta, especialmente para os alunos com uma nota média mais alta.

Palavras-chave: esquemas relacionais, ensino de multiplicação de frações, curva de regressão logística, avaliação do desenvolvimento educacional.

Résumé: Le nombre et ses opérations de base peuvent être conceptualisés dans un système général de relations. Les enfants ont besoin de construire un système de nombres au sein duquel ils peuvent additionner, soustraire, multiplier et diviser n'importe quel nombre rationnel. Les produits et les quotients peuvent être définis en termes de schémas relationnels généraux. Dans cette étude, nous examinons si les enfants des écoles élémentaires peuvent construire un système de nombres tel que la multiplication et la division des fractions sont basées sur la construction de schèmes relationnels généraux. Les groupes d'étudiants ne sont pas homogènes et les enfants progressent à des rythmes différents. Pour une évaluation fiable, les enseignants ont besoin de méthodes pour examiner les différences de développement et individuelles dans les représentations cognitives des concepts et des opérations mathématiques. Une courbe de régression logistique offre une visualisation du processus d'apprentissage en fonction des moyennes. L'analyse des éléments de multiplication et de division des fractions montre une amélioration de la probabilité de réponse correcte, en particulier pour les étudiants ayant une note moyenne plus élevée.

Mots clés: schémas relationnels, enseignement de la multiplication de fractions, courbe de régression logistique, évaluation du développement de l'éducation.

1. Introduction

The conceptual development of number and its basic operations (addition and multiplication) has constituted an essential part of research on cognitive development (Carpenter, Fennema, Franke, Levi & Empson, 2015; Empson & Levi, 2011; Empson, Levi, & Carpenter, 2011; Piaget, 1952; Lamon, 2005; Lortie-Forgues, Tian, & Siegler, 2015; Piaget, 1952; Piaget & Inhelder,1958; Siegler, et al., 2010; Siegler & Lortie-Forgues, 2015; Torbeyns, Schneider, Xin, & Siegler, 2015; Vygotsky, 1986). Scholastic education is one of the principal sources of the children's scientific and mathematical concepts and is also a powerful force in directing their development (Vygotsky, 1986). The main educational goal in elementary mathematics is that children develop mathematical descriptions and explanations and use mathematical tools to solve academic and real problems (Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (oecd, 2016). It has been proposed that elementary school children’s development of fraction knowledge (including decimals, percentages, ratios, rates, and proportions) seems to be especially important for overall mathematics achievement and later academic success. Moreover, children’s understanding of decimals simultaneously draws on their understanding of fractions. In addition to their importance for educational and occupational success, fractions are crucial for theories of numerical development (Siegler and Lortie-Forgues, 2015; Torbeyns, et al., 2015).

However, elementary school teachers and students tend to understand arithmetic as a collection of procedures, and students often are taught computational procedures with fractions without an adequate explanation of how or why the procedures work (Siegler, et al., 2010; Empson, et al., 2011). Although elementary school teaching focuses on both conceptual understanding and procedural fluency teachers should emphasise the connections between them (Siegler, et al., 2010). Academic tasks at elementary school create the necessary demands and conditions to conceptualise the number and its basic operations.

According to Vygotsky (1986), systematic learning plays a leading role in the conceptual development of elementary school children. Vygotsky upholds that the development of spontaneous concepts knows no systematisation and goes from the particular event, object or situation upward toward generalisations. In an opposite way, the development of mathematical and scientific concepts is the consequence of a systematic cooperation between the children and the teacher. The mathematical and scientific concepts, therefore, stand in a different relation to the events, objects or situations. This relation is only achievable in conceptual terms, which, in its turn, is possible only through a system of concepts. Vygotsky (1986) emphasises that the acquisition of academic concepts is carried out with the mediation provided by already acquired concepts. In general, Gergen (2009) contends that the meaning of a word is not contained within itself but derives from a process of coordinating words and that language (and other actions), in essence, gain their intelligibility in their social use.

In addition, Piaget (1952; Piaget & Inhelder, 1958) suggests that in formal thought there is a reversal of the direction of thinking of reality and possibility, and it is the reality that is now secondary to the possibility. Children conceive, for the first time, that the given facts form part of a set of possible transformations that has actually come about from a system of relationships. According to Piaget (1952), every totality is a system of relationships just as every relationship is a segment of totality. The possibilities entertained in formal thought are by no means arbitrary or equivalent to imagination freed of all control and objectivity. Quite to the contrary, the advent of possibilities must be viewed from the dual perspective of logic and physics; this is the indispensable condition for the attainment of a general form of equilibrium. Children recognise relations, which in the first instance they assume as real, in the totality of those which they recognise as possible.

The number and its basic operations can be conceptualised within a system of relations. At the beginning, certain aspects of objects are abstracted and generalised into the concept of number and the mathematical basic operations (addition and multiplication). However, mathematical concepts represent generalisations and schematic representations of certain aspects of numbers, not objects, and thus signify a new level of cognitive processes (Zapatera Llinares, 2017). This new processing level transforms the meaning of the first conceptualisations of number and its basic operations. This produces the construction of one general system of numbers.

Generalisations can be developed using different approaches. Children in the first courses of elementary school can develop concepts about fraction numbers through counting or measuring activities. Simona, Placab, Avitzurc, & Karad (2018) show how students can develop a measurement concept of fractions. Their proposal is consistent with the E-D approach developed by Davydov & Tsvetkovich and the Japanese text series, Tokyo Shoseki, developed by Fujii & Iitaka. From the perspective of the E-D curriculum, measurement is not just a basis for fraction numbers, but for numbers in general from the first elementary grades. The proposal is based on the idea that number should be developed as a general concept, and that any number, whole or fraction, does not require a change in the general basic concept.

In contrast to the counting and measuring cognitive activities, we focus on children’s understanding of fractions based on relational schemes. Our activities promote children's generalisation of multiplication and division computational procedures to include whole and fraction numbers in general schemes. The images children construct might imply measuring cognitive activities, but measuring does not play a central role in our learning sessions. The core of our programme is the concept of number as a relational scheme.

Our proposal is based on the construction of generalised conceptualisation of, at least, rational numbers and the development of generalised procedures to perform rational numbers mathematical operations.

1.1 Cognitive schemes of fractions and their basic operations

As a general rule, instruction in fraction numbers, i.e. a number that can be represented by an ordered pair of whole numbersa/b (Musser, Burger, & Peterson, 2008), and their basic operations begins with addition and subtraction of fractions with common denominators, proceeds to instruction in those operations with unequal denominators and to fraction multiplication, and then moves to fraction division. We propose that the best approach to present this subject is to begin with fraction multiplication and fraction division. That is because children need to know how to multiply and/or divide fractions in order to obtain equivalent fractions with the aim of adding, subtracting or comparing fractions with unequal denominators. Consequently, in this paper, we constrain our research to multiplication of rational numbers and its related operation, division. The focus of our inquiry is on children’s schemes that define multiplication as a mathematical process whereby a rational number multiplied by another rational number results in a third rational number.

1.2 Cognitive construction of rational numbers

Elementary school children do not discriminate between the set of natural numbers and the set of rational numbers. Numbers, in general, are signs or symbols representing an amount or total and they can be conceptually understood in relation to other numbers. Every natural number is specifically represented by a unique symbol (Siegler & Lortie-Forgues, 2015). However, in general, any number can be represented in a great variety of mathematical relationships. Vygotsky (1986) asserts that through the study of arithmetic, children learn that any number can be expressed in countless ways because the concept of any number contains also all of its relations to all other numbers. For example, a whole number can be represented as a fraction and hence has an infinite number of fraction equivalences (Musser, Burger & Peterson, 2008). The number one, for instance, can be expressed as the difference between any two consecutive numbers, or as any number divided by itself, or in a myriad of other ways. According to this relational perspective, every number can be represented by infinite expressions. The number 5 can be defined or represented as:

5 = 4 + 1 = 6 - 1 = 3 + 2 = 1 0 2 = 2 5 = 7 5 7

In view of this, we conclude that children’s cognitive structures conceptualising numbers constitute relational schemes. A relational scheme can be defined as any scheme whose essential characteristic or feature is a relationship between at least two concepts, objects or situations (Díaz-Cárdenas, Sankey-García, Díaz-Furlong, & Díaz-Furlong, 2014). In Vygotskian words, we cannot study concepts as isolated entities but we must study the "fabric" made of concepts. We must discover the connections between concepts based on the principle of the relation of generality, not based on either associative or structural relationship.

Teacher instruction relying on the typical mathematical tasks of elementary school create the conditions that engender children’s need to construct a system of numbers within which they can add, subtract, multiply and divide. Scholastic tasks like calculating the number which added to five equals three, or calculating the number which multiplied by five equals thirty one, constitute the basis for expanding the number system, restricted at first, to the positive integers to include the negative and rational numbers. Natural numbers are not closed under subtraction and they are not closed under division either. Therefore children need to expand the numbers system to include zero, negative numbers and fractions. At least, they need to understand and conceptualise the rational numbers (ℚ, from quotient). Within ℚ they can subtract and divide any number (except divide by zero). This number system includes a variety of relations in terms of comparisons and equivalences of spatial or temporal magnitudes and quantities (length, surfaces, volumes, units of weight or time) or abstract numbers.

In this paper we present data about a very important issue related to opposing approaches to the introduction of fraction multiplication and division. One research perspective that contends that fractions and decimals need to be treated differently from whole numbers, and a second approach, which we adopt, that is based on the construction of general relational schemes for any mathematical basic operation that combines two real numbers to form a single real number. In this study, we examine whether elementary school children can construct a system of numbers such that fraction multiplication and division are based on the construction of general relational schemes. We also want to test the hypothesis that children achieve an improvement on correct response probability, especially those students with a higher average mark.

1.3 Fraction multiplication

Research on the direction of effects of fraction arithmetic operations suggests that learner’ incorrect predictions about products and quotients result from the belief that multiplication yields answer greater than both factors, and that dividing yields answer smaller than the dividend (Siegler, et al., 2015; Graeber, Tirosh & Glover, 1989). This question depends on the particular case and it can be answered if the student understands the multiplication scheme or the division scheme in itself. Basically, students must develop a sound understanding of fraction operations so as to analyse and modify their misconceptions about multiplication and division (Greer, 1988). Therefore we need to help children to develop a reconceptualization of number that includes the fractional basic operations. In developing general cognitive schemes it is not a relevant issue if a product or quotient is greater o smaller than any of the factors or the division elements. Fraction multiplication and division must be developed as cases of general relational schemes and, basically, as a conceptual generalisation of these operations with natural numbers. Elementary school children can construct a system of numbers such that multiplication and division, products and quotients, are defined by every number comprised in the system.

Multiplication can be expressed by the words “multiplied by” or “times” (the corresponding Spanish words are “por” and “veces” respectively). An algebraic expression of a product c is a × b = c . This can be read as a times b or b times a equals c. Likewise, it can be transcribed as the product c results from taking a times the number b or taking b times the number a. In a similar way children can say that a product results from adding a number to itself a particular number of times.

To prevent students’ belief that multiplication should always yield answers larger than either factor we introduce gradually fraction multiplication exercises that result in products that can be at the same time greater than one of the factors and smaller than the other factor. Cognitive systems, according to Piaget (1975), never reach a final equilibrium point but they are evolving in a continuous process of progressive equilibration. Cognitive schemes are constantly modified by school exercises so they become able to give a comprehensive account of number multiplication and division. Elementary school children commonly learn to calculate a product that can be the result of taking:

Children learn multiplication and its properties multiplying whole numbers, the first multiplication case (a). Children’s understanding of fractions based on relational schemes can be introduced by (b) or (c) multiplications. They can conceptualise multiplication by fraction numbers as taking a whole number times a fraction number (b) or taking a fraction times a whole number (c). If we use the same numbers in both cases children have a fractional multiplication example of the Commutative Property for Number Multiplication ( 1 8 × 1 3 = 1 3 × 1 8 ) (see the section learning procedure). Finally, children must be able to take a fraction times another fraction, understanding that they can get a correct mathematical answer if they take a non-whole number times a fraction, that they can take a part of a part, for example, two fifths times five sevenths. Most elementary school children understand that multiplication computational procedures apply in the same way to fractions when they are provided with opportunities to solve multiplications involving fractions.

Problem solving in mathematics requires an understanding of the relations involved in a problem and developing a corresponding translation into a mathematical relation (Vygotsky, 1986). Children can be helped to quickly recognise patterns of information and to organise data in schemes and they will be able to develop relational schemes that generalise these math relations. Products and quotients can be defined in terms of relational schemes (Díaz-Cárdenas, et al., 2014). A general multiplication scheme must include any rational number (decimal or fraction). According to Empson and Levy (2011) children must think of a fraction as a number.

Product defined in relational terms
factor product xy is factor
y y times x x
y x times y x
y the y-ple of x x
y the x-ple of y x

In conceptualising different objects in a name or a category it is necessary to select a set of common properties or qualities and determine those that contrast them with other elements belonging to other categories (Díaz-Cárdenas, et al., 2014;Rogers & McClelland, 2004). Children understand that all four multiplications above-mentioned represent a mathematical operation that results from taking one number a number of times. One contrasting feature is the procedural knowledge that produces the resulting factor of:

  1. 1. Taking a whole number of times a whole number,
  2. 2. Taking a whole number of times a part of another number that is an equivalent operation to taking specific fraction times a whole number.
  3. 3. Taking specific fraction times a fraction number.

1.4 Fraction division

Children learn that there is a number that multiplied by 3 equals 9, and there is a number that multiplied by 3 equals 10. But if there is a Closure Property for Fraction Multiplication there must be a number that multiplied by 3 equals 10 and another number that multiplied by 3 equals 11 (see the section learning procedure). Here we can introduce the division of fractions. On the subject of division students also need to avoid some common misconceptions, and a significant number of children and their teachers believe that the quotient must be a whole number (Graeber, et al., 1989). They hardly represent the remainder as a fraction part of the quotient. On the other hand, incorrect responses to the direction of effects on division tasks are by-products of a misconstruction of products and quotients. Therefore, we begin by considering division as a mathematical process that results in dividing a rational number by another rational number that produces a third rational number named quotient and we basically apply the missing-factor approach (Musser, et al., 2008). This means that division consists of three mathematically related numbers: a dividend, a divisor, and a third number called the quotient or missing factor. The children´s task is to find the number that multiplied by the divisor equals the dividend and they can define division for every two numbers within only one general scheme for all rational numbers.

Quotient (q) defined in relational terms
dividend relationship divisor
y y contains/comprises/includes q times x x ≠ 0
y y equals q times x x ≠ 0
y q is the number that multiplied by x equals y x ≠ 0

1.5 School Assessment Analysis

The second, but no less important objective of this study, is based on elementary school teachers’ need for reliable assessment methods to examine developmental and individual differences in cognitive representations of fractions and in the effects of interventions aiming at improving conceptual knowledge of fractions.

The second, but no less important objective of this study, is based on elementary school teachers’ need for reliable assessment methods to examine developmental and individual differences in cognitive representations of fractions and in the effects of interventions aiming at improving conceptual knowledge of fractions.

Assessment as part of the learning process is very effective when it is designed to reflect the understanding of how students learn. It is important to know how students progress in learning academic procedures and content. Assessment is an essential ingredient both in research and education processes. A valid assessment system implies a model of student cognition and learning in a specific topic, a set of beliefs about the kinds of data that will provide evidence of students’ cognitive processes in learning, and an analysis and information processing for making sense of the evidence (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2018). Assessment design and analysis are becoming as essential as other elements of teaching in Mexico. Teachers must include in their didactic planning detailed rubrics. These must contain evaluation parameters and procedures for performance analysis.

In elementary school, children’s learning depends on different individual factors. Groups of students are not homogeneous and children progress at different rates. Therefore, when teachers base their analysis on group average achievement, they cannot see how students are differentially progressing.

The logistic function depicts the probability of success on an item as a function of a students’ specified parameter, i.e. it is possible to analyse learning progress in relation to any variable that can be evaluated with non-categorical scales. With this tool, teachers or researchers can perform basic item analysis in relation to an ability parameter based on academic grades, psychological test scores, or performance on a cognitive scale. To attempt a first approximation analysis, we selected average marks as the parameter that can be related to the probability of right answer to an item. We decided to study children´s average mark or grade as the ability parameter. Average mark is basically a socially defined index that represents academic performance, and this index is only one element of the universal set of social indexes designed to assess and analyse learning processes.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

Fifth graders attending two elementary middle-income schools in Puebla city, México, participated in this study (N =104). There were two fifth-grade groups studying in each school. One school pertains to the public school system and the other one is a private school. Only students with parental consent were included in the study. According to the official requirements of the Secretaría de Educación Pública (the Secretary of Education), fifth-grade children participating in this program had their tenth birthday during the year of the study. Tests and learning sessions were developed in the children´s schools. One group played a part as a control group and the other participated as the fraction multiplication and division learning group in each school. As the group A would be the learning group in the private school we decide to take the group B as the learning group in the public school. Therefore, we have two control groups and two learning groups (see table I).

Table I
Distribution by sex, academic group, and school system in control and learning groups
Treatment School Sex Group Total
A B
Learning groupPublicFemale2020
Male2020
PrivateFemale1010
Male99
SubtotalFemale102030
Male92029
Total194059
ControlPublicFemale1212
Male1313
Privatefemale1111
male99
Subtotalfemale121123
male13922
Total252045
Female total 53
Male total 51
Participant total 104

2.2 Learning procedure and methods of microgenetic analysis

The learning instruction period was necessarily brief because of our commitment to working the same learning sessions with the control group before the academic year finished. During the learning sessions, we asked children to write a verbal expression that makes visible a conceptual understanding of the fraction multiplication, as well as the standard mathematical expression and, when possible, to draw a picture or diagram representing the multiplication. When children calculate products that involve greater numbers they do not need to make a drawing (see Figure 1 as an example of tasks solved in school).

An example of one student´s multiplication exercises.
Figure 1
An example of one student´s multiplication exercises.

Sessions with the fractions multiplication/division learning groups were delivered in a whole-class arrangement in half-hour periods two times per week for three and a half weeks. The first author had charge of the learning sessions and the school teachers did not intervene in the teaching of multiplication or division of fraction. Control groups did not receive any special intervention. To prevent parents or teachers intervention in the multiplication and division learning process we did not assign any homework.

Some researchers have suggested that children make of errors that reflect inappropriate generalization from the corresponding whole number arithmetic procedures (Siegler & Lortie-Forgues, 2015; Lortie-Forgues, Tian, & Siegler, 2015, Siegler, Thompson, & Schneider, 2011). According to them an important factor that contributes to the difficulty that children commonly encounter with fraction arithmetic is the opposite direction of effects of multiplying and dividing positive fractions below and above one. Siegler & Lortie-Forgues affirm that understanding the direction of effects of multiplying and dividing proper fractions poses special problems for learners. Multiplying natural numbers always results in an answer greater than either multiplicand but multiplying two proper fractions invariably results in answers less than either multiplicand. Similarly, dividing by a natural number never results in an answer greater than the number being divided, but dividing by a proper fraction or decimal always does. Both an important number of students and some teachers show poor understanding of the directional effects of fraction and decimal multiplication and division (Siegler & Lortie-Forgues, 2015). These researchers recommend that understanding fractions requires recognizing that many properties of natural numbers are not properties of numbers in general. An instructional implication is that teachers and textbooks should emphasize that multiplication and division produce different outcomes, depending on whether the numbers involved are greater than or less than 1, and should discuss why this is true (Lortie-Forgues, Tian, & Siegler, 2015, Siegler, Thompson, & Schneider, 2011). For this reason, we designed school activities that give rise to the construction of a system of numbers such that fraction multiplication and division are based on the development of general relational schemes.

In addition, by definition a fraction multiplication can be expressed as: , but in order to avoid a simple mechanistic procedure we do not use this definition to solve fraction multiplications during learning sessions, and neither do we use the “invert-the-divisor-and-multiply” procedure for fraction division ( a b ÷ c d = a d b c ,with b≠0 and c≠0). In our programme children did not learn to multiply fractions in the traditional method for multiplication, whereby numerators and denominators of the multiplying fractions are treated as if they were independent multiplication problems with whole numbers.

Microgenetic methods offer a promising way to meet the challenges inherent in trying to understand change processes (Chen & Siegler, 2000, p.12). The brevity of the analyzed period allows us to assume that the observed effects will be largely a result of the interventions carried out, since the other social factors remain, on the whole, without significant changes. Obtaining a precise understanding of cognitive change requires observing such changes while they are occurring and to define the path of change, i.e. the sequence of knowledge states that the child passes through while gaining competence, constitutes a dimension that had proved useful in microgenetic studies (Fazio & Siegler, 2013). Our hypothesis is that children can go through the following path in learning fraction multiplication:

The comprehension activities that we applied to the different types of fraction multiplication were:

  1. Understanding and solving fraction multiplication word problems

  2. Drawing a picture or diagram representing fraction multiplication

  3. Understanding and solving fraction multiplication problems represented numerically

Consequently, we began to work with exercises like the following products (we use in the learning sessions examples not included in the evaluation tests):

2 × 1 3 =

2 × 1 5 =

2 × 1 7 =

Once these are read, respectively, as two times one-third equals, two times one-fifth equals, two times one-seventh equals, most children correctly answer that the respective products are two-thirds, two-fifths, and two-sevenths. Incidentally, in session discussions, children agree, at least some of them that two-thirds are greater than one-third and smaller than two wholes, i.e. they acknowledge that the product is at the same time greater than one factor and smaller than the other factor. We then calculate products that involve greater whole numbers. For example:

6 0 × 1 3 = 1 3 × 6 0 =

This kind of exercise let the children generalise the multiplication relational scheme applied before to greater whole numbers and to apply the associated commutative law for multiplication ( a × b = b × a ). These activities help students to recognize that n times a fraction equals n times the fraction. Therefore sixty times one third equals sixty thirds. But this is equivalent to saying one third times sixty or one third of sixty. Most fifth-grade children correctly answer that one third of sixty equals twenty. Therefore, students understand and arrive at the conclusion that sixty thirds equals twenty (see Fig. 1).

We worked immediately after on the multiplication of two fractions. Yet again, we used the word times (veces) to help children to apply the same product relational scheme when multiplying fractions. Children conceptualise multiplication by one half as a product that results from taking a half times of a fraction. In general, multiplying by one half represents dividing in two halves a fraction and taking away one half of the original fraction (see Fig. 1). The result produces fractions with a denominator equal to two times the original denominator. Therefore, one half times one third equals one sixth, and similarly, one half times one sixth equals one twelfth.

We finish working with the children in multiplication with fractions with a numerator greater than one. We tried to use simple justifications to build schematic relationships. Let´s examine two multiplication cases, 1 5 × 3 5 (a very similar fraction multiplication exercise to those analysed by Empson and Levy, 2011, p. 85), and 1 4 × 3 5 . Children, in the first instance, only need to understand the equivalence of these mathematical expressions to 3 5 × 1 5 and 3 5 × 1 4 respectively (commutative property of multiplication). Students can immediately construct, correspondingly, these multiplications as three times one fifth times fifteen and three times one fifth times one fourth, that correspond, in the same order, to the mathematical representations: 3 × 1 5 × 1 5 and 3 × 1 5 × 1 4 . The next step is to calculate one fifth times fifteen (5) or one fifth times one fourth ( 1 2 0 )(associative property). As a final point, children, easily, calculate three times those partial products and it reinforces the procedural knowledge to calculate the product of a whole number (3) times a unitary fraction ( 1 5 )[tres veces un quinto son tres quintos]. This kind of scholastic tasks helps children to consolidate and apply a general relational scheme of multiplication and two basic properties of this number operation.

As a final point, we briefly introduce fraction division basically applying the missing-factor approach in which the children’s task is to find the number that, multiplied by the divisor, equals the dividend (Musser, et al., 2008). Succinctly children calculate how many times the divisor is contained in the dividend. In the first instance, fraction division problems were designed with the aim of defining multiplication for any integer number and extending the system of numbers. Consequently, children conceptualise division within only one general scheme for all rational numbers. To understand the need of rational numbers children calculated the numbers that multiplied by three equals nine or twelve, which are two and three respectively. But which numbers multiplied by three equals ten and eleven? Children must use rational numbers to answer this question and the resulting sequence would be like this:

3 × 3 = 9

3 × 1 0 3 = 1 0

3 × 1 1 3 = 1 1

3 × 4 = 1 2

Children also can draw a picture or diagram representing every multiplication. For example, three times ten thirds equals ten can be represented as:


Therefore, the product sequence (9, 10, 11, 12) is completed if we introduce the numbers ( 1 0 3 ) and ( 1 1 3 ). To avoid some previously mentioned misconceptions about fraction division students calculated quotients resulting from the division of an integer by a fraction number. For instance, to divide seven by one half children must find a number that multiplied by one half equals seven, or a number whose half equals seven. The result derived from fraction multiplication schemes used before is 1 4 × 1 2 or . 1 2 × 1 4 It is important for children to analyse and to understand that the quotient resulting from dividing 7 ÷ 1 2 is not really greater than seven. The quotient is not an abstract and absolute 14. The meaning of this quotient is that fourteen times one half equals seven, i.e. fourteen halves are contained in seven wholes, and it does not mean that fourteen wholes or units are contained somehow in seven wholes or units. This procedure could also be approached as a division process that solves the question of how many halves are contained in seven units.

2.3 Pre- and post- assessments

The only way to find out how children learn is to study them closely while they are learning (Chen & Siegler, 2000). If we examine thinking before and after changes occur, we can identify those children that move between different levels or stages from those who do not move to an advanced one. Participants solved 5 items on a multiplication fraction problem; three of them were verbally represented (eg, How much is one third of 18?¿Cuánto es un tercio de 18?) while the rest were presented in a standard mathematical form (eg, how much is one-third of 18? ¿Cuánto es un tercio de 18?) while the rest were presented in a standard mathematical form (eg, what is 1 8 × 1 4 ? ¿Cuánto es 1 8 × 1 3 ?). The assessment also included 5 items on a division problem; this part once again contained three word items (eg, which number that multiplied by one half equals seven? ¿qué número multiplicado por un medio nos da siete?) and two numerical items (eg, find 7 ÷ 1 2 ¿Cuánto es 7 ÷ 1 2 ?). These fraction problems were administered before and after treatment, to both control and learning groups.

2.4 Data analysis

The logistic function describes the relationship between the probability of correctly answering an item and the corresponding examinee’s specified ability. The item response curve depicts the probability of success on an item as a function of a person’s specified parameter ability. We employ here the two-parameter logistic model based on the following function:

p θ = 1 1 + e - a ( θ - b )

where θ is an ability parameter, stands for the item difficulty (the required ability level for an individual to have a probability of 1 2 to respond correctly to an item), and b is the item discrimination according to item response theory (Fan & Sun, 2013; Baker, 2001). Item discrimination is an important index for assessing the quality of an item for differentiating students by ability levels on the basis of probability of successful response to an item (Wu, 2013; Baker, 2001). The unit on the ability parameter scale is known as logit (abbreviation for “log of odds unit” or logarithm of the odds). While the theoretical range of ability is from negative infinity to positive infinity, practical considerations usually limit the range of values from (-3 to +3 (Baker, 2001). Higher values of logit represent higher level of the attribute related to the correct answer probability (DeVellis, 2017). If the item response curve reach the point P θ = . 5 0 within the defined range (-3 to +3) the value of logit corresponds to the parameter defined as the item difficulty according to the item response theory. Three of the authors of this paper developed an application to be used in the common ® Microsoft Excel program (a copy of this macro can be obtained freely by request from the authors. As a result, we get an item response curve that constitutes the best fit of children´s item responses to a logistic function calculated using a genetic algorithm.

From the perspective of item response theory students who obtained the correct answer are of higher average ability than students who obtained the incorrect answers. However, an item response curve also could be interpreted as showing that students of higher average ability have a better chance of being successful on an item than students of lower average ability. We assumed that item difficulty is not fixed but it changes as learning develops. Abilities are not fixed, and successful item response probability, at least for academic assessments, varies as a function of cognitive development and learning.

There are two extreme cases for which the IRT ability estimation procedure fails. First, when children do not answer correctly any of the items, and second when students answer the test items without any mistakes. In both cases, it is impossible to obtain an ability estimate for the examinee (Baker, 2001). The item response curve is either very low or very high and essentially flat. If there are no answer differences between students calculation of an ability parameter is not possible. Therefore the item contributes very little to our knowledge about children’s ability, as it does not differentiate between students with lower versus higher ability (Fan & Sun, 2013). It is not uncommon to find those situations at elementary school, either at the beginning of a learning process or at the final stage of this process.

In the case of learning fractions, children show a tendency to answer incorrectly most numerically represented items of a diagnostic test before instruction of this topic begins. As the course progresses, children’s responses to fraction mathematics diverge. We analise here an ability parameter that teachers could bring into play that is related to fraction knowledge progress: children´s average mark or grade. In this way, children’s item responses along with children´s average mark, as an ability parameter, are the basis for analysing the development of those responses with a logistic function. An item response curve is a useful aid to visualise, item by item, children’s progress as a function of their average mark and teachers can compare, per item, correct answer probabilities P θ , odds ratio ( P θ / 1 -P θ ), and logit by average mark level ((Díaz-Furlong, Díaz-Furlong, & Díaz-Cárdenas, 2017; Pardo & Ruiz, 2012).

3. Results

Commonly teachers use overall group test scores to analyse academic improvements. The simplest way to do this is to apply a Student’s two-tailed paired t-test (Pardo, Ruiz, & San Martín, 2009). Table IIpresents the results of a dependent samples t-test to compare general pre-test/post-test scores of learning groups (teachers do not usually compare their results with control groups) on multiplication items (word and numerical forms), division items (word and numerical forms), and total fraction items (word, numerical, and final total). There are statistically significant differences between any general scores, except for word multiplication items. Fifth-grade children can solve correctly word problems that involve fraction multiplication calculations, although they do not perform any multiplication. They calculate one-third of 18 calculating the third part of 18. On the other hand, they are not able, in general, to make any operation to calculate a quotient for a fractional division problem at the time of the pre-test.

Table II
Mean differences, standard deviations, standard error differences, and Student’s two-tailed paired t-test values of learning group pre-test/post-test scores.
Paired Differences t df Sig. (two-tailed)
Mean Difference Std. Deviation Std. Error Difference 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower Upper
word multiplication items -0.1271.4020.189-0.5060.252-0.673540.504
numerical multiplication items -1.1640.9580.129-1.423-0.905-9.011540.000
multiplication items -1.2911.8730.252-1.797-0.785-5.113540.000
word division ítems -0.9641.1860.160-1.284-0.643-6.027540.000
numerical division ítems -0.7090.7860.106-0.922-0.497-6.692540.000
division ítems -1.6731.8060.244-2.161-1.185-6.869540.000
word ítems -1.0912.0570.277-1.647-0.535-3.933540.000
numerical ítems -1.8731.4410.194-2.262-1.483-9.639540.000
fraction ítems -2.9643.0550.412-3.789-2.138-7.195540.000

3.1 Word multiplication items

We present here an alternative form of item examination to analyse, item by item, improvements as a function of previous academic marks. The item response curve constitutes a very useful visual device to appreciate changes in correct response probability related to academic mark levels. In the first place, it is important to assess children’s knowledge of verbal expressions related to fraction multiplication. Fifth-grade children have informal fraction knowledge, they already understand a fraction as a part of a collection, and they are able to answer a problem expressed in words (how much one-third of 18 are or what is third of 18; ¿cuánto es un tercio de 18). Figure 2 shows the item response curves for the word item: Students show a relatively good performance in solving this word fractional problem. There is no noticeable change either in control or learning groups.


Figure 2

Item response curves depicting the probability of success on the item how much is one-third og 18? as a function of children´s average academic mark, taken as the ability parameter. The horizontal axis is the ability level: from the left to the right, the ability level goes from lower (-3) to higher (+3) levels. The unit on this scale is known as logit (abbreviation for “log of odds unit”). Control groups graphs (above) and learning groups curves (below) show no distinctive differences between pre-test (left) to post-test (right) answers.

We performed a one-way repeated-measures anova on these data using Tukey’s HSD (Honestly Significant Difference) test for post hoc comparisons (Pardo & San Martín, 2010). This analysis compares the item responses by average mark with the aim of validating the item response curve result. As expected, post hoc comparisons of the item responses of the learning groups show that there is no significant difference for any average mark level (see Table III).

Table III
Mean differences, standard error, and significance values by average mark resulting from Tukey’s HSD of learning group pre-test/post-test answers to the item How much is one-third of 18?
Average mark Mean differences Standard Error Sig. 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower Upper
5.5.000.3041.000-1.071.07
6.5-.125.2151.000-.88.63
7.0.286.2300.997-.521.09
7.5.000.2151.000-.76.76
8.0-.167.2481.000-1.04.71
8.5-.600.2720.689-1.56.36
9.0.143.2301.000-.67.95
9.5-.200.1921.000-.88.48
* The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level.

3.2 Numerical multiplication items

The analysis of responses to problems expressed in numerical terms gives a different result. Most of the children give an incorrect answer at pretest when the problem is represented numerically ( 1 3 × 1 8 ). Based on our learning sessions children might solve this question as follows:

What is 1 8 × 1 3 ?= What is 1 3 × 1 8 ?

1 8 × 1 3 = 1 8 3 = 6 = 1 3 × 1 8 = 6 (standard mathematical expression)

18 times one third equals eighteen thirds that equals six wholes (verbal expression) (solution drawing)


Figure 3 presents the item response curves for the numerical item: What is 1 8 × 1 3 ?Here, the learning groups reveal an improvement on correct response probability, especially for students with a higher average mark.


Figura 3

Item response curves depicting the probability of success on the item What is 1 8 × 1 3 ? as a function of children´s average academic mark, taken as the ability parameter. Control groups graphs (above) show no distinctive differences between pre-test (left) to post-test (right) answers, whereas learning groups curves (below) show post-test (right) marked improvements in the probability of correct answer, in particular for those children with the higher average academic mark.

In a similar way, we apply a Tukey’s HSD test to compare the item responses by average mark. In this case, post hoc comparisons of the item responses revealed that students with an average mark greater than or equal to 8.0 showed a significant improvement on item response (p < .05). Groups with an average mark less than or equal to 7.5 showed no significant difference on item response for any average mark level (see table IV). The item response curve indicates that children with these lower average marks are less likely to answer successfully to the item: What is 1 8 × 1 3 ?. This means that for these children this question remains a difficult item.

Table IV
Mean differences, standard error, and significance values by average mark resulting from Tukey’s HSD of learning group pre-test/post-test answers to the item What is 18×1/3?
Average mark Mean differences Standard Error Sig. 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower Upper
5.5-.250.227.999-1.05.55
6.5-.375.160.598-.94.19
7.0-.429.172.482-1.03.18
7.5-.500.160.145-1.06.06
8.0-.833*.185.002-1.49-.18
8.5-1.000*.203.000-1.71-.29
9.0-1.000*.172.000-1.60-.40
9.5-.900*.144.000-1.41-.39
*. The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level.

Teachers also can compare correct answer probability and odds ratio by average mark. Table V presents those data for control and learning groups.

Table V
Control groups’ and learning groups’ correct answer probability and its correspondent odds ratio by average mark level to the item What is 18 × 1/3?
Correct answer probability Odds ratio
control groups learning groups control groups learninggroups
average mark pre-test post-test pre-test post-test pre-test post-test pre-test post-test
5.50.000.000.000.500.000.000.001.00
6.50.000.000.000.330.000.000.000.50
70.000.000.110.250.000.000.130.33
7.50.330.000.130.630.500.000.141.67
80.000.000.000.670.000.000.002.00
8.50.000.000.000.670.000.000.002.00
90.250.250.000.860.330.330.006.00
9.50.250.000.000.820.330.000.004.50

We performed a covariate analysis (ancova) to statistically evaluate the effect of the average mark on item response. ancova represents a recommended data statistical analysis in our case and it is a combined application of anova and regression analysis (Kline, 2009; Pardo & Ruiz, 2012). Here we take the average mark as a covariate, i.e. a variable that predicts the outcome but is ideally unrelated to the independent variable (Kline, 2013). The obtained results indicate (once the effect of the average mark was controlled): 1) a significant improvement on item response to the items What is 1 8 × 1 3 ?( F = 3 1 . 5 2 , p = . 0 0 0 ) and What is 1 3 × 1 8 ?( F = 3 4 . 4 2 , p = . 0 0 0 ); 2) there is a significant difference between pre-test post-test answers to both items ( F = 3 8 . 3 3 , p = . 0 0 0 and F = 3 6 . 6 9 , p = . 0 0 0 and respectively); 3) there is a significant difference between the learning condition groups and the control groups in post-test correct response to those items ( F = 5 9 . 8 9 , p = . 0 0 0 and F = 5 9 . 0 2 , p = . 0 0 0 respectively); and, 4) the average mark as covariate is related to differences on pre-test post-test answers to both items ( F = 6 . 9 2 , p = . 0 1 0 and F = 8 . 4 6 , p = . 0 0 5 respectively).

Item response curves depicting the probability of success on the item
Figure 4
Item response curves depicting the probability of success on the item

Which is the number that multiplied by one half equals seven? as a function of children´s average academic mark, taken as the ability parameter. Control groups graphs (above) show no distinctive differences between pre-test (left) to post-test (right) answers, whereas learning groups curves (below) show post-test (right) marked improvements in the probability of correct answer, in particular for those children with the higher average academic mark.

3.3 Numerical division items

On the other hand, to solve fraction division items, even when they are represented verbally, implies difficult concepts for many children. Figure 4 displays the item response curves for control and learning groups corresponding to the item: Which is the number that multiplied by one half equals seven? The increase in correct response probability is lower than in the multiplication item reviewed before.

If teachers perform a Student’s two-tailed paired t-test to analyse the differences observed in this item they can observe that the learning group, in general, showed better results at post-test assessment: t ( 5 4 ) = - 6 . 4 6 5 , p = . 0 0 0 . But the item response curve indicates that only children with greater average marks have a better post-test performance. Teachers can corroborate it with a Student’s two-tailed paired t-test applied to each average mark. This method produces the calculations displayed in Table VI and they confirm that only children with average marks greater than or equal to 9.0 showed a significant improvement on item response (p < .01).

Table VI
Mean differences, standard deviations, standard error differences, and Student’s two-tailed paired t-test values by average mark level of learning group pre-test/post-test answer to the item Which is the number that multiplied by one half equals seven?
Paired Differences t df Sig. (two-tailed)
Average mark Mean Difference Std. Deviation Std. Error Difference 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower Upper
9.5-.600.516.163-.969-.231-3.6749.005
9.0-.714.488.184-1.166-.263-3.8736.008
8.5-.400.548.245-1.080.280-1.6334.178
8.0-.500.548.224-1.075.075-2.2365.076
7.5-.375.518.183-.808.058-2.0497.080
7.0-.286.488.184-.737.166-1.5496.172
6.5-.375.518.183-.808.058-2.0497.080
5.5.000.000.000

4. Conclusions

We present a microgenetic study that focuses on specific proximal influences in cognitive change (Siegler & Chen, 1998). The learning instruction period was brief, three and a half weeks; therefore, we can assume that most important social factors affecting learning remained unchanged for learning and control groups except our instruction sessions with the first groups.

As mentioned above, in this paper we present data about a very important issue related to opposing approaches to the introduction of multiplication or division of fractions: One point of view that contends that fractions and decimals need to be treated differently from whole numbers, and a second approach, which we adopt, that is based on the construction of general relational schemes for any basic mathematical operation. We propose here that fraction multiplication and division must be developed as relational schemes and, basically, as a conceptual generalisation of these operations with natural numbers. We have designed activities in order to develop a general relational scheme of the multiplication and division of numbers. We do not agree that children must understand that fraction multiplication and division produce different outcomes, depending on whether the numbers involved are greater than or less than 1. We could promote the construction of two different sets of numbers if we teach children that understanding fractions requires recognizing that many properties of natural numbers are not properties of numbers in general (Lortie-Forgues, Tian, & Siegler, 2015, Siegler, Thompson, & Schneider, 2011), and this would produce the need of a different procedural scheme to multiply or divide fraction numbers.

On the other hand, we avoid mechanistic procedures ( a b ÷ c d = a d b c ; the “invert-the-divisor-and-multiply” procedure) because using them children can develop a fraction definition as if it is composed of two whole numbers (numerator and denominator) that must be conceptualised separately in multiplication and division, obscuring the fraction concept as a unity.

Our approach probably allows elementary school children to construct a system of numbers such that multiplication and division, products and quotients, are defined by every number comprised in the system. At the elementary school level, that system corresponds to rational numbers, ℚ. Within that system, every number can be expressed as the product or quotient of, at least, two other numbers. That is, every two numbers of the system can be related according to the definitions of multiplication and division to a number termed product or quotient. For example, 15 can be represented as the product of one-half times ten, or the quotient of five divided by one third, i.e. the number that that multiplied by one third equals five.

The authors attempt a first approximation analysis and select students’ average mark as the parameter that could related to the probability of a right answer to an item. We describe here an analysis procedure that permits a visualisation of the learning process as a function of average marks and we present data that supports the validity of this approach. According to the social constructionism approach (Gergen, 2001), we assume that all learning is an active process of social construction. Average marks and item difficulty can be modified by social interaction processes. Children can improve or worsen their academic marks as a result of different social factors. The probability of success in answering an item depends on the average mark of the student. Average mark is basically a socially defined index that represents academic performance, and this index is only one element of the universal set of social indexes designed to assess and analyse learning processes. The item difficulty depends on the learning process. Children pick up a good deal of expertise in the learning process and consequently, the item difficulty diminishes substantially. The logistic function depicts the probability of success on an item as a function of a person’s specified parameter, i.e. it is possible to analyse learning progress in relation to any variable that can be evaluated with non-categorical scales. Consequently, it is necessary to research further significant relationships among other relevant social factors and the probability of success in an item. Here a tool is offered to analyse the relationships between some of these variables and the methods of assessment that teachers apply in their courses.

In this study, we examined whether elementary school children can construct a system of numbers such that fraction multiplication and division are based on the construction of general relational schemes. Learning groups increase their performance following this kind of program. There are statistically significant differences between any general scores, except for word multiplication items. Finally from a Bayesian sequential analysis, we obtained that the results are statistically robust.

Agradecimientos

ADF acknowledge PRODEP for the grant of the NPTC project (BUAP-PTC-457, DSA / 103.5 / 16/11069); ADF acknowledge the Sistema Nacional de Investigadores (SNI) and CONACyT. ADF and AFDC acknowledge to the authorities of the department of psychology, BUAP.

Referencias

Baker, F.B. (2001). The Basics of Item Response Theory. 2nd ed. ERIC Clearinghouse on Assessment and Evaluation

Carpenter, T.P., Fennema, E., Franke, M.L., Levi, L., & Empson, S.B. (2015). Children's mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction. 2nd ed. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Chen, Z., & Siegler, R.S. (2000). Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers’ and older children’s thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, v–96.

DeVellis, R.F. (2017) Scale Development. Theory and Applications. 4th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE

Díaz-Cárdenas, A.F., Sankey-García, M.R., Díaz-Furlong, A., Díaz-Furlong, H.A. (2014) Educación por competencias y desarrollo social del procesamiento cognitivo. In Patiño Tovar, H. y López Cortés, A. (comp.) Prevención y Evaluación en Psicología. (pp. 75 – 97). México: Manual Moderno.

Díaz-Furlong, H.A., Díaz-Cárdenas, A.F., Díaz-Furlong, A. (Eds.) (2017). Literacy and cognition: Development and learning. Berlin: Verlag/Publisher. LAP Lambert Academic Publishing.

Empson, S.B., & Levi, L. (2011). Extending children’s mathematics: Fractions and decimals. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Empson, S.B., Levi, L., and Carpenter T.P. (2011). The Algebraic Nature of Fractions: Developing Relational Thinking in Elementary School. In J. Cai, E. Knuth (Eds.). Early Algebraization, Advances in Mathematics Education, pp. 409 – 428. Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. doi 10.1007/978-3-642-17735-4_22

Fan, X., & Sun, S. (2013). Item Response Theory. In Timothy Teo (Ed.), Handbook of Quantitative Methods for Educational Research (pp. 45–67). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

Fazio, L.K. & Siegler, R.S. (2013). Microgenetic Learning Analysis: A Distinction without a Difference. Human Development, 56: 52 – 58. doi: 10.1159/000345542

Gergen, K. J. (2001). From mind to relationship: The emerging challenge. Education Canada-Toronto-, 41(1): 8 -11.

Gergen, K.J. (2009). Pragmatics and pluralism in explaining human action. Behavior and Philosophy, 37, 127-133

Graeber, A.O., Tirosh, D., & Glover, R. (1989). Preservice Teachers' Misconceptions in Solving Verbal Problems in Multiplication and Division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20 (1), 95 – 102.

Greer, B. (1988). Nonconservation of multiplication and division: Analysis of a symptom. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior.

Kline, R.B. (2009). Becoming a behavioral science researcher: a guide to producing research that matters. New York: The Guilford Press.

Kline, R.B. (2013). Beyond significance testing : statistics reform in the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Lamon, S.J. (2005). Teaching fractions and ratios for understanding: Essential content knowledge and instructional strategies for teachers. 2nd ed. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers

Lortie-Forgues, H., Tian, J., & Siegler, R.S. (2015). Why is learning fraction and decimal arithmetic so difficult? Developmental Review, 38, 201 – 221. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.008

Musser, G.L., Burger, W.F., & Peterson, B.E. (2008). Mathematics for elementary teachers: A contemporary approach. 8th ed. Jefferson City: John Wiley & Sons

National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. (2018). How People Learn II: Learners, Contexts, and Cultures. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/24783

oecd (2016). pisa 2015 Assessment and Analytical Framework: Science, Reading, Mathematic and Financial Literacy. Paris: oecd Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264255425-en

Pardo, A., Ruiz, M.A., & San Martín, R. (2009). Análisis de datos en ciencias sociales y de la salud I. Madrid: Editorial Síntesis.

Pardo, A., & Ruiz, M.A. (2012). Análisis de datos en ciencias sociales y de la salud III. Madrid: Editorial Síntesis.

Pardo, A., & San Martín, R. (2010). Análisis de datos en ciencias sociales y de la salud II. Madrid: Editorial Síntesis.

Piaget, J. (1952). The Child's Conception of Number. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd.

Piaget, J. (1975). L’Équilibration des Structures Cognitves. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1958). The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence. New York: Basil Books, Inc.

Rogers, T.T. & McClelland, J.L. (2004). Semantic cognition. A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press

Siegler, R., Carpenter, T., Fennell, F., Geary, D., Lewis, J., Okamoto, Y., Thompson, L., & Wray, J. (2010). Developing effective fractions instruction for kindergarten through 8th grade: A practice guide (NCEE #2010-4039). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from whatworks.ed.gov/ publications/practiceguides.

Siegler, R. S., Thompson, C. A., & Schneider, M. (2011). An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development. Cognitive Psychology, 62, 273–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.03.001

Siegler, R.S. and Lortie-Forgues, H. (2015) Conceptual Knowledge of Fraction Arithmetic. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, (3), 909 – 918. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000025

Siegler, R.S., & Chen, Z. (1998) Developmental differences in rule learning: A microgenetic analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 273 – 310. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0686

Simona, M.A., Placab, N., Avitzurc, A. & Karad, M. (2018). Promoting a concept of fraction-as-measure: A study of the Learning Through Activity research program. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 52: 122 – 133. doi: 10.1016/j.jmathb.2018.03.004

Torbeyns, J., Schneider, M, Xin, Z., & Siegler, R.S. (2015). Bridging the gap: Fraction understanding is central to mathematics achievement in students from three different continents Learning and Instruction, 37, 5 -13. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.03.002

Vygotsky L. S. (1986) Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press

Wu, M. (2013) Using Item Response Theory as a Tool in Educational Measurement. In M. Mo Ching Mok (ed.), Self-directed Learning Oriented Assessments in the Asia-Pacific, Education in the Asia-Pacific Region: Issues, Concerns and Prospects 18. (pp.: 157 – 185), Dordrecht: Springer. DOI 10.1007/978-94-007-4507-0_9

Zapatera Llinares, A. (2017). Cómo alumnos de educación primaria resuelven problemas de generalización de patrones. Una trayectoria de aprendizaje. Revista Latinoamericana de Investigación en Matemática Educativa, 20 (3): 87 - 114. doi: 10.12802/relime.18.2114

HTML generado a partir de XML-JATS4R por Redalyc